Why do we forget important things




















Try this well-known demonstration first used by researchers Nickerson and Adams. Once you are done, compare your drawing to an actual penny. Are you surprised by how poorly you recalled what the back of a penny looks like?

While you probably had a good idea about the overall shape and color, the actual details were probably pretty fuzzy. Since you don't actually need to know what the back of a penny looks like to differentiate it from other coins, you only really focus on the information you do need—the overall size, shape, and color of the coin.

You aren't able to recall what the back of a penny really looks like because that information was never really encoded into memory in the first place. Other researchers have suggested that sometimes information is actually present in memory, but that it cannot be recalled unless retrieval cues are present.

For example, remembering the details of your first date with your spouse might be easier if you smell the same scent that your partner was wearing on that first date.

The retrieval cue the scent was present when that memory was created, so smelling it again can trigger the retrieval of those memories. Forgetting is simply a part of life. Numerous theories explain how and why we forget. In many situations, several of these explanations might account for why we cannot remember. The passage of time can make memories more difficult to access, while the abundance of information vying for our attention can create competition between old and new memories.

Still, we can work to become better at recalling information. Ever wonder what your personality type means? Sign up to find out more in our Healthy Mind newsletter. Aronowitz S. Retrieval is central to the distinctive function of episodic memory. Behav Brain Sci. Murre JM, Dros J. Replication and analysis of Ebbinghaus' forgetting curve.

PLoS One. A comparison of immediate serial recall and immediate serial recognition. Can J Exp Psychol. The cost of learning: Interference effects in memory development. J Exp Psychol. Troyer A. Serial position effect. Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. Springer, Overlearning hyperstabilizes a skill by rapidly making neurochemical processing inhibitory-dominant. Nat Neurosci. Rasch B, Born J. About sleep's role in memory. Physiol Rev.

The visual nonverbal memory trace is fragile when actively maintained, but endures passively for tens of seconds. Mem Cognit. Decay theory of immediate memory: From Brown to today Q J Exp Psychol Hove.

Long-term memory for a common object. Cog Psychol. Less we forget: retrieval cues and release from retrieval-induced forgetting. Tulving E. Cue-dependent forgetting. American Scientist. Your Privacy Rights. To change or withdraw your consent choices for VerywellMind. At any time, you can update your settings through the "EU Privacy" link at the bottom of any page. These choices will be signaled globally to our partners and will not affect browsing data.

We and our partners process data to: Actively scan device characteristics for identification. I Accept Show Purposes. But that memory is soon forgotten typically by the next day. Something erases it, and the evidence suggests dopamine is responsible for the forgetting, too. But mushroom body neurons possess two distinct molecular antennas that respond to dopamine; one of those antennas or receptor molecules initiates memory formation, the other promotes erasure.

Whether dopamine promotes or erases memory may depend on the context, including prevailing biochemical conditions and how active the mushroom body neuron is at the time.

Fruit flies remember to fear an odor if its presence is accompanied by an electric shock. That memory forms because the shock stimulates the release of the molecule dopamine from nerve cells linked to mushroom body neurons at the same time that the odor triggers a cellular signal via calcium.

Recall fades over time as, in the absence of the odor, lower levels of dopamine stimulate another dopamine receptor molecule DAMB , leading to a weakening of the memory. In any case, the erasing process involves a protein known as Rac1, which plays a part in structuring synapses.

Restructuring synapses in response to Rac1 may be responsible for weakening engrams, some studies indicate. Blocking Rac1 activity, for example, helps to extend how long memories persist. Rac1 may also be involved in a second forgetting mechanism, driven by the birth of new nerve cells the process known as neurogenesis. Studies in rats have found that new neurons integrated into existing neural circuits can restructure the circuitry. Such changes in connections might make memories harder to access, Frankland said at the neuroscience meeting.

Animal studies have shown that disrupting neurogenesis preserves memories, while high levels of neurogenesis drive forgetting. Whether that form of forgetting is important in humans remains unknown, as the amount of neurogenesis in adult humans is still an unsettled question. How and when those processes operate can depend on various factors, such as physical activity, stress and sleep.

But sleep may also aid memory by suppressing the processes that drive forgetting, Davis and Zhong point out. A study published in Cell found evidence that sleep inhibits release of the dopamine forgetting signal to mushroom body neurons. If forgetting is the key to how the brain successfully processes the massive data input it encounters each day — as research accumulated so far suggests — then flaws in the forgetting process could plausibly contribute to brain disorders, Davis and Zhong note.

Deficits in the ability to forget may be involved in autism spectrum disorders, for instance. Certainly the powerful and debilitating memories of post-traumatic stress disorder reflect an inability to forget disturbing experiences. Unwanted, repetitive invasive memories are a feature of some psychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia. And the inability to forget cues associated with addictive drug use impairs recovery from substance abuse.

On the plus side, better insight into the biology of forgetting could help identify drugs capable of enhancing needed memories while disposing of undesirable ones.

But such benefits may appear only after much more research, Davis said at the neuroscience meeting — speaking at a rather sparsely attended session. According to the authors, they found support for the idea that a memory can take the form of two different representations in the brain; familiarity or recollection.

This is like when you feel you recognize a face, that guy looks so familiar , but you cannot put your finger on where you know the person from. In this process you recognize that guy , and you remember his name or other defining details. Our Canadian research team suggests that these two types of memory representations act differently, and look different in the brain. Decay should be a major source of their forgetting.

By contrast, familiarity-based memories, supported by extrahippocampal structures… [are] sensitive to interference. So, what have we learned? Why we forget seems to depend on how a memory is stored in the brain. Things we recollect are prone to interference. Things that feel familiar decay over time. The combination of both forgetting processes means that any message is unlikely to ever remain exactly the way you wrote it.

The views expressed are those of the author s and are not necessarily those of Scientific American. Julia Shaw is a research associate at University College London. She is also a, speaker, and author of the international best-selling book The Memory Illusion: Remembering, Forgetting, and the Science of False Memory , released in 14 languages in Already a subscriber?

Sign in.



0コメント

  • 1000 / 1000